Tag: Brain Science

  • How the Brain Works, Flies, and the FlyBase Online Data Repository

    Fruit flies have a complex enough nervous system, with brains composed of about 100,000 neurons, to support some behaviors found in humans and other mammals. Contrast that with the human brain that has somewhere in the neighborhood of one hundred billion neurons and add the fact that the fruit fly is one of the most studied organisms in genetics and development and you can see why our little insect friend is an excellent candidate for clarifying principles governing how the brain works.

    A new paper published in Science describes research that uses a combination of sophisticated genetic, anatomical, and physiological techniques to discover a pair of neurons that control a fundamental fly behavior. It seemed the perfect opportunity for me to take a peek at the latest in fly neuroscience. Also, I wanted to see if I could find tools based on Semantic Web technologies that would leverage my knowledge of mammalian brains to help me understand the fly nervous system.

    As it turns out the paper “Two Pairs of Neurons in the Central Brain Control Drosophila Innate Light Preference” (published October 22, 2010 in Science) is a veritable alphabet soup of fly jargon. “…TeTxLC expression driven by NP394-Gal4 was excluded from the NP394-neurons…” Perfect!

    The first step described in the paper was to screen a large number of fly larva with a genetic manipulation that, when expressed at a connection between nerve cells, would turn off communication from one neuron to the next (turn off the synaptic release of neurotransmitter). Normally fly larvae avoid light. Most of the genetically manipulated larvae continued to avoid light but some lost any light preference and a few actually preferred light.

    The team focused on the larvae that preferred light and used labeling techniques to highlight the cells responsible for the switch from light avoiding to light loving fly larvae. They narrowed the cells responsible to two pairs of neurons in the supraesophageal ganglion. At this point I was ready for a quick, preferably visual, orientation to the fly nervous system and how the supraesophageal ganglion fits in.

    I was aware that FlyBase, an online data repository of fly genes and genomes, was instrumental in developing a fly (Drosophila melanogaster) gross anatomy ontology so I took a look at their website (see Figure 1)

    Note: The FlyBase Anatomy (FBbt) ontology is hosted on the Open Biological and Biomedical Ontologies [OBO] website and is available in Web Ontology Language [OWL] in addition to OBO format.

    Figure 1. The FlyBase website includes a QuickSearch area.

    I was able to search for and find the supraesophageal ganglion on the FlyBase website. To try it yourself go to FlyBase and, in the QuickSearch area, open the drop-down menu next to the “Data Class” label by clicking on the up-down arrows. Scroll down and select “controlled vocabularies” and then enter “supraesophageal ganglion” into the “Enter text” box. The page should look something like in Figure 2.

    Figure 2. Search the FlyBase control vocabularies for supraesophageal ganglion.

    Note: If you open the drop-down menu for “CV Hierarchy” in the QuickSearch area you’ll see at least 5 ontologies listed. The search for the supraesophageal ganglion will use the FlyBase Anatomy ontology.

    Click the Search button and you should see a page returned that looks something like in Figure 3.

    Figure 3. The results from a search for the supraesophageal ganglion.

    Click on the term “supraesophageal ganglion” and information about the term is displayed like in Figure 4.

    Figure 4. A description in addition to other information displayed after clicking on supraesophageal ganglion.

    The page describes the supraesophageal ganglion but this is probably most helpful to a fly specialist. I’ll need to take some time and find diagrams. Nevertheless, I’ve found the FlyBase site to be useful even at the anatomical level. I entered “pdf” into the QuickSearch area and FlyBase returned a list of terms that included “Pdf neuron.” Pdf is a factor associated with specific neurons in the fly larva known to receive input from photoreceptors. The pair of neurons that the research team found (NP394-neurons) are adjacent to Pdf neurons in the supraesophageal ganglion and are involved in light preference behavior.

    FlyBase doesn’t claim to be an anatomical atlas. It’s a repository for genes and genomes and there are useful links with anatomical terms within this context. However, I’d love to be able to use a virtual fly nervous system similar to the virtual mouse brain in the Whole Brain Catalog. I haven’t found anything like that. Have you?

    Other related blog posts:

    NeuronBank: Neuronal Circuit Online Data Repository

    Whole Brain Catalog: the Google Earth for the Brain

    Whole Brain Catalog: Brain Cells and Molecules

    Bio-Commons a Global Challenge

    Whole Brain Catalog: Visualizing Neural Network Activity

  • Mental Activity Influences Brain Biochemistry

    You’re going to a house that you’ve never been to before. At a stoplight you glance at the address and then, when the light turns green, you look for the number. You’re using what is technically called working memory to maintain mental access to the house number.

    Many studies have shown that working memory may be improved through intensive training, which is encouraging since aging reduces working memory capacity. There are also neurological and psychiatric disorders that impact working memory.

    The neurotransmitter dopamine is known to play a central role in working memory. Researchers asked if intensive mental activity due to training could affect dopamine. Specifically they looked at the concentration of two types of dopamine receptors in the brain. They looked to see if there may be a change in the number of D1 receptors in the cerebral cortex or D2 receptors in subcortical brain regions.

    The paper titled “Changes in Cortical Dopamine D1 Receptor Binding Associated with Cognitive Training” was published February 6, 2009 in Science.

    Volunteers went through 14 hours of training to improve working memory over a 5 week period. Using brain imaging techniques (functional magnetic resonance imaging) the research team showed that improved working memory was associated with a decrease in the number of available D1 receptors.

  • Is the Conscious Decision an Illusion? Pursuing Goals Unconsciously

    Most of us consider that conscious will is the starting point of pursuing a goal. Recent discoveries challenge this view. The recent paper “The Unconscious Will: How the Pursuit of Goals Operates Outside of Conscious Awareness” published July 2, 2010 in Science reviews research that indicates that setting goals and then pursuing them may all happen without the involvement of conscious awareness.

    I recall my surprise when Benjamin Libet presented data that showed people beginning to act to fulfill a goal, in this case by moving their finger, before they made the conscious decision to act. The lecture was more than 25 years ago but I recall wondering if our conscious decisions were sometimes simply after-the-fact explanations for actions and goals we had already carried out without conscious awareness. Evidence accumulated since then suggests that this may be the case.

    The authors review recent research showing that social situations and other stimuli act to influence and control an individual’s pursuit of goals without the individual’s conscious awareness. But how can people determine whether it’s worth pursuing a given goal and invest effort in attaining it without the involvement of consciousness?

    Conscious awareness of rewards is not needed for the pursuit of goals to occur is the conclusion reached by the authors of this review. They point out that the brain processes and represents behaviorally relevant information in such a way that the pursuit of a goal can be controlled by the social situation without conscious awareness of the activation and operation of the goal.